R

Quantifying three years of a long distance relationship

I read two really useful guides to processing text data recently; an analysis of Trump’s tweets to work out whether it’s him or an intern sending them, and a sentiment analysis of Pride and Prejudice. Three years of a long distance relationship means that I have a nice big corpus of Whatsapp messages between my girlfriend and me, so I did the romantic thing and quantified some of our interactions in R. Also, this required quite a bit of text munging in Excel first, which turned out to be far quicker and easier than using regex in this case.

First of all, let’s look at when we text each other throughout the day. We’re in different time zones, but only by an hour, and since texts are inherently dependent – one text is overwhelmingly likely to lead to another pretty soon after – I haven’t adjusted the times.

text no by hour of day.png

Our texting activity represents our general activity pretty well; nothing much going on until about 7am, then a slow start to the day, a bit of a post-lunch dip, and then an evening peak when we’re more likely to be out and about doing things.

We can also have a look at how many messages we send each other, and how that’s changed over time:

text no by date.png

We’ve sent each other a fairly similar number of texts per day throughout the long distance period, but it looks pretty bad on me that I have consistently sent fewer texts than her…

…or does it? When I plot the length of each text sent, I consistently write longer messages:

text length by date.png

So, there’s two distinct texting styles here; I write longer messages less frequently, she writes shorter messages more frequently. The other thing I like about the text length graph is that you can see the times when we’ve been together and not texted each other that much; three weeks in November 2014 when I was running experiments in London, three weeks around Christmas 2015, and a load of long weekends throughout. It’s not that we don’t text each other at all then, it’s more that those texts tend to be stuff like “have we got milk?”, or simply “pub?”.

Plotting log likelihood ratios of how much each of us uses each word in comparison to the other also captures our texting styles:

top 20 words each (no names).png

For example, we both use the word /ha/ to express laughter, but I spell it “ha” and she spells it “hah”. Likewise, “til” and “till” as abbreviations for “until”, and I seem to use “somebody” while she uses “someone”.

If we filter out equivalent words and proper names (like the pubs, supermarkets, and stations we go to most often), another difference in dialogue style appears:

top 10 words each (no proper names).png

I am apparently a lot more conversational; I write out interjections (hmm, oooh, hey, ohhh) and reactions (fuck’s comes from for fuck’s sake, hoera comes from the Dutch phrase hiep hiep hoera, and boourns comes from, erm, The Simpsons). Apart from hhmmm, she doesn’t write interjections or contextual replies at all. Apart from the interjections and replies, my main thing is adjectives; she tends towards nouns and verbs.

The next step is sentiment analysis. If I plot log likelihood bars for each sentiment, I seem to be an atrociously negative person:

sentiment error bars.png

…but this, I think, is more a problem with the way sentiment analysis works in the syuzhet and tidytext packages using NRC sentiment data. Each word in the NRC corpus has a given value, 0 or 1, for a range of sentiments, and this sentiment analysis style simply adds it up for each word in a given set.

Because of that, it doesn’t really capture the actual sentiment behind the way we’re using these words. Let’s look at the main words driving the differences in each sentiment:

sentiment log likelihood words.pngFor me, a lot of my disgust and anger is coming from the word damn. If I was texting damn! every time I stubbed my toe or something, perhaps that would be accurate; but in this case, a lot of the time I write damn is in sympathy, as in exchanges like:

“My computer crashed this afternoon and I lost all the work I’d done today”
“Damn, that’s horrible”

Meanwhile, the word coop is actually me talking about the coöp / co-op, where I get my groceries. I’m not talking about being trapped, either physically or mentally.

The same goes for my girlfriend being more positive. With words like engagement and ceremony, she’s not joyous or anticipatory about her own upcoming nuptials or anything; rather, several of her colleagues have got engaged and married recently, and most of her uses of the words engagement and ceremony are her complaining about how that’s the only topic of conversation at the office. As for assessment, council, and teacher, she works in education. These are generally neutral descriptions of what’s happened that day.

So, I was hoping to be able to plot some sentiment analyses to show our relationship over time, but either it doesn’t work for text messages, or we’re really fucking obtuse. I think it might be the former.

Instead, I’ll settle for showing how much we both swear over time:

expletives per month.png

Each dot represents the number of occurrences per month of a particular expletive. I’m clearly the more profane here, although I do waver a bit while she’s fairly consistent.

More importantly is how we talk about beer a similar amount:

beer per month.png

Since couples who drink together stay together (or in the words of this study, “concordant drinking couples reported decreased negative marital quality over time”), I think this bodes pretty well for us.

Advertisement
Standard
R, Uncategorized

Visualising football league tables

I was looking at the Premiership league table today, and it looks like this:

current league table

It’s pretty informative; we can see that Leicester are top, Aston Villa are bottom, and that the rest of the teams are somewhere in between. If we look at the points column on the far right, we can also see how close things are; Villa are stranded at the bottom and definitely going down, Leicester are five points clear, and there’s a close battle for the final Champions League spot between Manchester City, West Ham, and Manchester United, who are only separated by a single point.

Thing is, that requires reading the points column closely. If you take the league table as a simple visual guide, it doesn’t show the distribution of teams throughout the league very well. If you say that Stoke are 8th, that sounds like a solid mid-table season… but what it doesn’t tell you is that Stoke are as close to 4th place and the Champions League as they are to 10th place, which is also solid mid-table. A more visually honest league table would look something a little like this*:

current league table dragged about a bit

*definitely not to scale.

Screen-shotting a webpage and dragging things about in MS Paint isn’t the best way to go about this, so I’ve scraped the data and had a look at plotting it in R instead.

Firstly, let’s plot each team as a coloured dot, equally spaced apart in the way that the league table shows them:

League position right now

(colour-coding here is automatic; I tried giving each point the team home shirt colours, but just ended up with loads of red, blue, and white dots, which was actually a lot worse)

Now, let’s compare that with the distribution of points to show how the league positions are distributed. Here, I’ve jittered them slightly so that teams with equal points (West Ham and Manchester United in 5th and 6th, Everton and Bournemouth in 12th and 13th) don’t overlap:

League points right now

This is far more informative. It shows just how doomed Aston Villa are, and shows that there’s barely any difference between 10th and 15th. It also shows that the fight for survival is between Norwich, Sunderland, and Newcastle, who are all placed closely together.

Since the information is out there, it’d also be interesting to see how this applies to league position over time. Sadly, Premiership matches aren’t all played at 3pm on Saturday anymore, they’re staggered over several days. This means that the league table will change every couple of days, which is far too much to plot over most of a season. So, I wrote a webscraper to get the league tables every Monday between the start of the season and now, which roughly corresponds to a full round of matches.

Let’s start with looking at league position:

League position over time

This looks more like a nightmare tube map than an informative league table, but there are a few things we can pick out. Obviously, there’s how useless Aston Villa have been, rooted to the bottom since the end of October. We can also see the steady rise of Tottenham, in a dashing shade of lavender, working their way up from 8th in the middle of October to 2nd now. Chelsea’s recovery from flirting with relegation in December to being secure in mid-table now is fairly clear, while we can also see how Crystal Palace have done the reverse, plummeting from 5th at the end of the year to 16th now.

An alternative way of visualising how well teams do over time is by plotting their total number of points over time:

League points over time

This is visually more satisfying than looking at league position over time, as we can see how the clusters of teams in similar positions have formed. Aston Villa have been bottom since October, but they were at least relatively close to Sunderland even at the end of December. Since then, though, the gap between bottom and 19th as opened up to nine points. We can also see how Leicester and Arsenal were neck and neck in first and second for most of the season, but the moment when Leicester really roared ahead was in mid-February. Finally, the relegation fight again looks like it’s a competition between Norwich, Sunderland, and Newcastle for 17th; despite Crystal Palace’s slump, the difference between 16th and 17th is one of the biggest differences between consecutive positions in the league. This is because Norwich, Sunderland, and Newcastle haven’t won many points recently, whereas Swansea and Bournemouth, who were 16th and 15th and also close to the relegation zone back in February, have both had winning streaks in the last month.

One of the drawbacks with plotting points over time is that, for most of the early part of the season, teams are so close together that you can’t really see the clusters and trends.

So, we can also calculate a ratio of how many points a team has compared to the top and bottom team at any given week. To do this, I calculated the points difference between top and bottom teams each week, and then calculated every team’s points as a proportion of where they are.

For example, right now, Leicester have 66 points and Aston Villa have 16. That’s a nice round difference of 50 points across the whole league. Let’s express that points difference on a scale of 0 to 1, where Aston Villa are at one extreme end at 0 and Leicester are at the other extreme end at 1.

Tottenham, in 2nd, have 61 points, or five points fewer than Leicester and 45 points more than Aston Villa. This means that, proportionally, they’re 90% along the points difference spectrum. This means they get a relative position of 0.9, as shown below:

Relative league position over time

This is a lot more complicated, and perhaps needlessly so. It reminds me more of stock market data than a football league table. I plotted it this way to be able to show how close or far teams were from each other in the early parts of the season, but even then, the lines are messy and all over the place until about the start of October, when the main trends start to show. One thing that means is that however badly your team are doing in terms of points and position, there’s little use in sacking a manager before about November; there’s not enough data, and teams are too close together, to show whether it’s a minor blip or a terminal decline. Of course, if your team are doing badly in terms of points and position and playing like they’ve never seen a football before, then there’s a definite problem.

To make it really fancy/silly (delete as appropriate), I’ve plotted form guides of relative league position over time. Instead of joining each individual dot each week as above, it smooths over data points to create an average trajectory. At this point, labelling the relative position is meaningless as it isn’t designed to be read off precisely, but instead provides an overall guide to how well teams are doing:

Relative league position over time smooth narrative (span 0.5)

Here, the narratives of each team’s season are more obvious. Aston Villa started out okay, but sank like a stone after a couple of months. Sunderland were fairly awful for a fairly long time, but the upswing started with Sam Allardyce’s appointment in October and they’ve done well to haul themselves up and into contention for 17th. Arsenal had a poor start to the season, then shot up, rapidly to first near the end of the year, but then they did an Arsenal and got progressively worse from about January onwards. Still, their nosedive isn’t as bad as Manchester City’s; after being top for the first couple of months, they’ve drifted further and further down. It’s more pronounced since Pep Guardiola was announced as their next manager in February, but they were quietly in decline for a while before that anyway. Finally, looking at Chelsea’s narrative line is interesting. While they’ve improved since Guus Hiddink took over, their league position improvement is far more to do with other teams declining over the last couple of months. Four teams (Crystal Palace, Everton, Watford, and West Brom) have crossed Chelsea’s narrative line since February.

I don’t expect these graphs to catch on instead of league tables, but I definitely find them useful for visualising how well teams are doing in comparison to each other, rather than just looking at their position.

Standard
Cricket, R

Bigger isn’t always better – the case of the first innings in cricket

I’ve got an unsubstantiated hunch (the best kind of hunch!) about cricket. Well, not just one, I have loads, but this particular hunch is about the first innings of a cricket match, and that bigger isn’t always better.

I greatly enjoyed following England’s first innings against South Africa in the second Test in Cape Town. But, even with the high run rate while Stokes was smashing it everywhere, I was convinced that the higher that first innings got, the less likely we’d be to win it. This goes against the received wisdom in cricket, which is that the bigger the first innings score, the better it is.

So, I’ve had a look at all first innings scores in Tests from 1990 until now (there’s just over a thousand of them). Here’s simple density plot of the distributions of runs scored in the first innings per match result:

density plot of runs

What this seems to show is that there’s a limited sweet spot from just over 380 runs to about 500 runs where a win is the most likely result. Once a team scores over about 500 runs in the first innings, the most likely match result is a draw.

Part of that is probably because of how much time posting a huge first innings takes out of the game. What happens when we look at runs scored vs. balls taken in the first innings?

scatter plot of runs and balls simple

There’s a green cluster in the middle between about 350 and 550 runs and between about 700 and 800 balls. That, I reckon, is the sweet spot for the perfect first innings: scoring a high but not massive number of runs, without taking too much time. England took 755 balls (125.5 overs) in their first innings in Cape Town, so a win was still just about the most likely result there… but, this may just be an exception. We’ll see.

Here’s the same plot with some lines showing a run rate of 2, 3, and 4 runs per over (the steeper the line, the greater the run rate):

scatter plot of runs and balls

Visually, I’m convinced the sweet spot of 380-500 runs at a decent run rate is obviously there. So, let’s try looking at some simple percentages by comparing scores between 380-500 runs with scores over 500 runs, where runs are scored at over 3.5 runs an over:

Run rate over 3.5, runs between 380 and 500
won draw lost        = 62.32% win rate
43     16     10          = 2.69 win:draw ratio

Run rate over 3.5, runs over 500
won draw lost        = 54.29% win rate
57     47      1           = 1.21 win:draw ratio

The win rate goes down slightly for the higher scores, and the win:draw ratio goes down too. i.e. even if you’re scoring well, going beyond 500 just makes the draw more likely and doesn’t actually help your chances of winning.

But, that’s not quite a fair comparison. I said earlier that if you’re going to score more runs, you have to do it at a higher run rate, so comparing all scores above 3.5 an over isn’t exactly fair. Let’s now compare a good score at a good run rate with a high score at a high run rate. Again, I’m taking a good score to be 380-500 and a high score to be over 500. In terms of run rate, I’m quantifying a good run rate as between the mean run rate of all innings and the mean plus one standard deviation (i.e. between 3.13 and 3.72 runs per over), and a high run rate as above the mean plus one standard deviation (i.e. above 3.72 runs per over).

So, is a score of 380-500 at 3.13-3.72 runs per over better than a score of 500+ at 3.72+ ?

380-500 runs at 3.13-3.72 RPO (mean runs: 438 , mean RPO: 3.40)
won draw lost        = 56.10% win rate
46    20     16          = 2.3 win:draw ratio

500+ runs at 3.72+ RPO (mean runs: 587, mean RPO: 4.90)
won draw lost        = 57.14% win rate
44    32     1             = 1.375 win:draw ratio

…the lower, slower score isn’t better, but it isn’t worse either. The likelihood of winning stays the same; the only difference is that batting on makes losing much less likely and drawing much more likely.

This is really counterintuitive, and I find it hard to wrap my head around the fact that scoring 438 at 3.4 an over is about as likely to result in a win as scoring 587 at 4.9 an over. One possibility is that the matches which feature high first innings scores are played on absolute roads, like in the 1997 Colombo snoozeathon between India and Sri Lanka, meaning that a high second innings score is also pretty likely. Therefore, you’d expect the first and second innings scores to correlate in matches where the first innings was 500+ runs at 3.72+ RPO… but they don’t (r=0.07, p=0.52). Nor do the first and second innings scores correlate in matches where the first innings was between 380-500 runs at 3.13-3.72 RPO (r=-0.15, p=0.18). The only indication that a massive first innings score may mean that the pitch is easier to bat on is that the mean second innings score in response to a massive first innings score is 346.90, while the mean second innings score in response to a good first innings score is 307.09. A t-test between the two set of second innings scores is “relatively significant” (as an ever-hopeful colleague of mine used to say) with a p-value of 0.07, but that doesn’t cut it. This is another mystery for another blog post.

Right, back to looking at just the first innings scores and win rate. One last way of exploring this is by creating a matrix of win rates in bins of runs scored and run rate.

I’ve put all innings into bins of 50 runs and bins of 0.5 RPO. This means that every square in the following graphs is represented by a set of matches where that many runs have been scored at that rate. It’s only done for bins with at least five matches in (because you can’t really extrapolate from things where only one or two matches have happened, as that leads to a lot of 0% and 100% win rates).

This graph visualises the win rate per bin; the darker the green, the greater the likelihood of winning based on that kind of first innings:

rough matrix of runs, RPO, win rate - five matches or more, cropped

But what if, instead of plotting the simple win likelihood for all bins, we plot the most likely result based on that bin, along with the likelihood of that result? In this graph, the colour represents the top result – win, draw, or loss – and the intensity of that colour represents the likelihood – the more intense the colour, the more likely that result:

rough matrix of runs, RPO, top result, rate, cropped

In both matrices, the sweet spot with the most green and the most intense green falls within 400 and 500 runs… although it turns out that in terms of overall win likelihood, the best first innings is to score between 500 and 550 runs, scored at over 4 runs per over.

Ultimately, what this shows is that batting on past 500 or so makes losing the match hugely unlikely (but definitely not impossible), so if safety first is your watchword, have at it. However, if you want to win a Test match, there’s not much point in batting on past 500 or so in the first innings, 550 at most, no matter how fast you score (and if you do decide to go for the big imposing total, you’d better hurry up about it). Ben Stokes might have set a load of records, but with a bit of statistical sleuthing, he’d have realised it was pointless because his batting blitz was actually just making it harder for England to win.

Why bother creating these incredible cricketing memories when the statistics say hold back?

…because it’s much more entertaining. If you focus on the statistics all the time, you end up with a team like England under Peter Moores, where nobody knows anything before they’ve looked at the data. Fair enough, then.

Standard
R, Science in general

scatterplot / dotplot / losttheplot

I’m not sure how to game search engine optimisation algorithms, but hopefully you’ll end up here if you’ve googled “things that are better than histograms” or “like scatter plots but with groups and paired and with lines” or “Weissgerber but in R not Excel” or something similar.

Anyway. Weissgerber et al. (2015) have a fantastic paper on data visualisation which is well worth a read.

(tl;dr version: histograms are dishonest and you should plot individual data points instead)

Helpfully, Weissgerber et al. include instructions for plotting these graphs in MS Excel at the end should you wish to give it a go. But, if MS Excel isn’t your bag, it’s easy enough to try in R…

…apart from the fact that nobody really agrees on what to call these plots, which makes it really hard to search for code examples online. Weissgerber et al. refer to them as scatterplots, but in most people’s minds, scatterplots are for plotting two continuous variables against each other. Other writers refer to them as dotplots or stripplots or stripcharts, but if you don’t know the name, you don’t know that this is what you’re looking for, and all you can find is advice on creating different graphs from the ones you want.

JEDI KNIGHT - these aren't the scatterplots you're looking for

As an example, here’s some of my own data from a behavioural task in which participants had to remember things in two different conditions. The histogram with 95% confidence intervals makes it fairly clear that participants are more accurate in condition one than condition two:

accuracy for each condition in percent

The scatterplots / dotplots / whateverplots also show the distribution of the data quite nicely, and because it’s paired data (each participant does both conditions), you can draw a line between each participant’s data point and make it obvious that most of the participants are better in condition one than in condition two. I’ve also jittered the dots so that multiple data points with the same value (e.g.the two 100% points in condition_one) don’t overlap:

accuracy for each condition in percent - jitterdots

It’s easy to generate these plots using ggplot2. All you need is a long form or melted dataframe (called dotdata here) with three columns: participant, condition, and accuracy.

dotdata$condition<- factor(dotdata$condition, as.character(dotdata$condition))
# re-order the levels in the order of appearance in the dataframe
# otherwise it plots it in alphabetical order
 
ggplot(dotdata, aes(x=condition, y=accuracy, group=participant)) +
  geom_point(aes(colour=condition), size=4.5, position=position_dodge(width=0.1)) +
  geom_line(size=1, alpha=0.5, position=position_dodge(width=0.1)) +
  xlab('Condition') +
  ylab('Accuracy (%)') +
  scale_colour_manual(values=c("#009E73", "#D55E00"), guide=FALSE) + 
  theme_bw()
Standard
Cricket

Using R to calculate better cricket statistics… or, how to revolutionise the way we slag off Ian Bell.

Have you ever been bothered by the idea of career batting averages, how it doesn’t reflect a player’s form, and how it’s unfair to compare averages of cricketers who’ve played over a hundred tests to cricketers who’ve played maybe thirty since one bad innings will damage the experienced cricketer’s average way less than the relative newcomer?

Well, you’re not alone. I’ve always thought that cricinfo should report a ten-innings rolling average. Occasionally you get a stat like “Cook is averaging 60 or so in the last few matches” or whatever, but there’s no functionality on cricinfo or statsguru to be able to look that up.

Enter R. R is a free open-source statistical programme that I normally use for my ERP research, but it’s also the next best thing after Andy Zaltzman for settling arguments about cricket statistics.

I’ve written some R code which can take any cricketer on cricinfo and spit out a ten-innings rolling average to show fluctuations in form. Plotting it with ggplot2 can show a player’s peaks and troughs as compared to their career average, and can hopefully be used as a much more objective way of saying whether or not somebody’s playing badly.

Alastair Cook has been a lightning rod for criticism in the last couple of years. He scored heavily in his first few matches as England captain, and for a little while it seemed as though captaincy would improve his batting, but then he went into a long slump. He recently broke his century drought, and people are divided over whether he’s finally hitting form again or whether this is a dead cat bounce on an inevitable decline. Some people take his last five Tests and say he’s back; others take his last year or two in Tests and say he’s lost it. What is missing from all the misspelled derision in the comments under any article about Cook is a ten-innings rolling average and how it changes over time.

Cook

Alastair Cook: rolling and cumulative averages

This graph shows Cook’s peaks and troughs in form quite nicely. The big one in the middle where he averaged about 120 over ten innings is a combination of his mammoth 2010-11 Ashes series and the home series against Sri Lanka where he scored three centuries in four innings. His recent slump can be seen in the extended low from his 160th innings and onwards, where his rolling average went down to below 20. Now, though, it’s clear that not only has he regained some form, he’s actually on one of the better runs of his career.

Similarly, it seems like commentators and online commenters alike feel like Gary Ballance should be dropped because he’s on a terrible run of form. Certainly, he’s had a few disappointing innings against the West Indies and New Zealand lately, but is his form that bad?

Gary Ballance: rolling and cumulative averages

Gary Ballance: rolling and cumulative averages

…no, no it isn’t. He’s still averaging 40 in his last ten innings.

If anything, it’s Ian Bell who should be dropped because of bad form:

Bell

Bell’s average has had a few serious drops recently, going down to 20 after a poor Ashes series in Australia (along with pretty much every other England player too), rebounding a bit after a healthy home series against India, and then plummeting back down to 20 after two bad series against West Indies and New Zealand. Unlike Cook, however, Bell never seems to stay in a rut of bad form for very long… but that never stops his detractors from claiming he hasn’t been good since 2011.

The missing bit in the cumulative average line, by the way, is from where Bell averaged a triple Bradman-esque 297 after his first three innings against West Indies and Bangladesh, which were 70, 65*, and 162*.

The forthcoming Ashes series also raises the interesting comparison of Joe Root and Steven Smith, two hugely promising young batsmen both at their first real career high points. Smith in particular is seen as having had an excellent run of form recently and has just become the #1 ranked Test batsman. Most cricket fans online seem to think that there’s no contest between Smith and Root, with Smith being by far and away the better batsman…

Root and Smith

…but it appears that there’s not actually much to choose between them. If anything, Root has had the highest peak out of the two of them, averaging 120 over ten innings against India last summer and the West Indies more recently (this is in fact comparable to Alastair Cook’s peak against Australia in 2010-11, but has attracted far less attention). He’s dropped a little since, but is still averaging a more than acceptable 85. Smith’s current rolling average of 105 is also very impressive, and it’ll be fascinating to see how he gets on in this series.

If you are interested in calculating and plotting these graphs yourself, you can follow the R code as below.

Firstly, if you don’t use them already, install and run the following packages:

install.packages('gtools')
install.packages('plyr')
install.packages('ggplot2')
install.packages('dplyr')
install.packages('XML')
require('gtools')
require('plyr')
require('ggplot2')
require('dplyr')
require('XML')

The next step is to create a dataframe of innings for each player. You can do this by going to any player’s cricinfo profile, and then clicking on “Batting innings list” under the statistics section. Take that URL and paste it in here like so:

# Joe Root innings
url = "http://stats.espncricinfo.com/ci/engine/player/303669.html?class=1;template=results;type=batting;view=innings"
Root.tables = readHTMLTable(url,stringsAsFactors = F)
Root.full = Root.tables$"Innings by innings list"

This creates a fairly messy dataframe, and we have to tidy it up a lot before doing anything useful with it. I rolled all the tidying and calculating code into one big function. Essentially, it sorts out a few formatting issues, then introduces a for loop which loops through a player’s innings list and calculates both the cumulative and ten-innings rolling averages at each individual innings (of course, the first nine innings will not return a ten-innings rolling average), and then puts the dataframe into a melted or long format:

rollingbattingaverage <- function(x) {
 
  x$Test <- x[,14]            # creates new column called Test, which is what column 14 should be called
  x <- x[,c(1:9, 11:13, 15)]  # removes 10th column, which is just blank, and column 14
 
  x$NotOut=grepl("\\*",x$Runs) #create an extra not out column so that the Runs column works as a numeric variable
  x$Runs=gsub("\\*","",x$Runs)
 
  #Reorder columns for ease of reading
  x <- x[,c(1, 14, 2:13)]
 
  #Convert Runs variable to numeric variables
  x$Runs <- as.numeric(x$Runs)
 
  #This introduces NAs for when Runs = DNB
  x <- x[complete.cases(x),] 
 
  rolling <- data.frame(innings = (1:length(x$Runs)), rollingave = NA, cumulave = NA)
  names(rolling) <- c("innings", "rolling", "cumulative")
 
  i = 1
  z = length(x$Runs)
  for (i in 1:z) {
    j = i+9
    rolling[j,2] = sum(x$Runs[i:j])/sum(x$NotOut[i:j]==FALSE)
    rolling[i,3] = sum(x$Runs[1:i])/sum(x$NotOut[1:i]==FALSE)
  }
 
  #because of the j=i+9 definition and because [i:j] works while [i:i+9] doesn't, 
  #creates 9 extra rows where all are NA
 
  x <- rolling[1:length(x$Runs),] #removes extra NA rows at the end
 
  melt(x, id="innings") 
 
}

Then I have another function which sorts out the column names (since changing the names of a function’s output is kind of tricky) and adds another column with the player’s name in it so that the player dataframes can be compared:

sortoutnames <- function(x) {
  x$player = deparse(substitute(x))
  allx <- list(x)
  x <- as.data.frame(lapply(allx, 'names<-', c("innings","type", "average", "player")))
}

Now we can plot an individual player’s rolling and cumulative averages:

plotplayer <- function(x) {
  myplot <- ggplot(data=x, aes(y=average, x=innings, colour=type))
  myplot+geom_line()+scale_y_continuous(limits=c(0, 200), breaks=seq(0,200,by=10))
}

The next function isn’t really necessary as a function since all it does is rbind two or more dataframes together, but it makes things easier and neater in the long run:

compareplayers <- function(...) {
  rbind(...)
}

And finally, we need to create functions for various types of graphs to be able to compare players:

plotrolling <- function(x){
  myplot <- ggplot(data=x[x$type=="rolling",], aes(x=innings, y=average, colour=player))
  myplot+geom_line()+scale_y_continuous(limits=c(0, 200), breaks=seq(0,200,by=10))
}
 
plotcumulative <- function(x){
  myplot <- ggplot(data=x[x$type=="cumulative",], aes(x=innings, y=average, colour=player))
  myplot+geom_line()+scale_y_continuous(limits=c(0, 200), breaks=seq(0,200,by=10))
}
 
plotboth <- function(x){
  myplot <- ggplot(data=comparisons, aes(x=innings, y=average, colour=player, size=type))
  myplot+geom_line()+scale_size_manual(values=c(0.6,1.3))+scale_y_continuous(limits=c(0, 200), breaks=seq(0,200,by=10))
}
 
plotrollingscatter <- function(x){
  myplot <- ggplot(data=x[x$type=="rolling",], aes(x=innings, y=average, colour=player))
  myplot+geom_point()+scale_y_continuous(limits=c(0, 200), breaks=seq(0,200,by=10))
}

Now that all the functions exist, you can get the information quickly and easily; just find the correct URL for the player(s) you want, paste it in the bit where the URL goes, and then run the functions as follows:

Root <- rollingbattingaverage(Root.full)
Root <- sortoutnames(Root)
plotplayer(Root)
comparisons <- compareplayers(Root, Smith)
plotrolling(comparisons)
plotcumulative(comparisons)
plotboth(comparisons)
Standard
EEG/ERP

Putting the graph into electroencephalography

ERPists – click to jump to the main point of this blog, which is about plotting measures of confidence and variance. Or just read on from the start, because there’s a lot of highly proficient MS Paint figures in here.

UPDATE! The paper where this dataset comes from is now published in Collabra. You can read the paper here and download all the raw data and analysis scripts here.

ERP graphs are often subjected to daft plotting practices that make them highly frustrating to look at.

ERPing the derp

Negative voltage is often (but not always) plotted upwards, which is counterintuitive but generally justified with “oh but that’s how we’ve always done it”. Axes are rarely labelled, apart from a small key tucked away somewhere in the corner of the graph which still doesn’t give you precise temporal accuracy (which is kind of the point of using EEG in the first place). And finally, these graphs are often generated using ERP programmes then saved as particular file extensions, which then get cramped up or kind of blurry when resized to fit journals’ image criteria. This means that a typical ERP graph looks something a little like this:

typical erp graph

…and the graph is supposed to be interpreted something a little like this:

erp intuitive 2

…although realistically, reading a typical ERP graph is a bit more like this:

erp context

Some of these problems are to do with standard practices; others, due to lack of expertise in generating graphics; and more still are due to journal requirements, which generally specify that graphics must conform to a size which is too small to allow for proper visual inspection of somebody’s data, and also charge approximately four million dollars for the privilege of having these little graphs in colour because of printing costs despite the fact that nobody really reads actual print journals anymore.

Anyway. Many researchers grumble about these pitfalls, but accept that it comes with the territory.

However, one thing I’ve rarely heard discussed, and even more rarely seen plotted, is the representation of different statistical information in ERP graphs.

ERP graphs show the mean voltage across participants on the y-axis at each time point represented on the x-axis (although because of sampling rates, it generally isn’t a different mean voltage for each millisecond, it’s more often a mean voltage for every two milliseconds). Taking the mean readings across trials and across participants is exactly what ERPs are for – they average out the many, many random or irrelevant fluctuations in the EEG data to generate a relatively consistent measure of a brain response to a particular stimulus.

Decades of research have shown that many of these ERPs are reliably generated, so if you get a group of people to read two sentences – one where the sentence makes perfect sense, like the researcher wrote the blog, and one where the final word is replaced with something that’s kind of weird, like the researcher wrote the bicycle – you can bet that there will be a bigger (i.e. more negative) N400 after the kind of weird final words than the ones that make sense. The N400 is named like that because it’s a negative-going wave that normally peaks at around 400ms.

Well, that is, it’ll look like that when you average across the group. You’ll get a nice clean average ERP showing quite clearly what the effect is (I’ve plotted it with positive-up axes, with time points labelled in 100ms intervals, and with two different colours to show the conditions):

standard N400

But, the strength of the ERP – that it averages out noisy data – is also its major weakness. As Steve Levinson points out in a provocative and entertaining jibe at the cognitive sciences, individual variation is huge, both between different groups across the world and between the thirty or so undergraduates who are doing ERP studies for course credit or beer money. The original sin of the cognitive sciences is to deny the variation and diversity in human cognition in an attempt to find the universal human cognitive capabilities. This means that averaging across participants in ERP studies and plotting that average is quite misleading of what’s actually going on… even if the group average is totally predictable. To test this out, I had a look at the ERP plot of a study that I’m writing up now (and to generate my plots, I use R and the ggplot2 package, both of which are brilliant). When I average across all 29 participants and plot the readings from the electrode right in the middle of the top of the head, it looks like this:

Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + NO GUIDE)

There’s a fairly clear effect of the green condition; there’s a P3 followed by a late positivity. This comes out as hugely statistically significant using ANOVAs (the traditional tool of the ERPist) and cluster-based permutation tests in the FieldTrip toolbox (which is also brilliant).

But. What’s it like for individual participants? Below, I’ve plotted some of the participants where no trials were lost to artefacts, meaning that the ERPs for each participant are clearer since they’ve been averaged over all the experimental trials.

Here’s participant 9:

ppt09 Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + NO GUIDE)

Participant 9 reflects the group average quite well. The green line is much higher than the orange line, peaking at about 300ms, and then the green line is also more positive than the orange line for the last few hundred milliseconds. This is nice.

Here’s participant 13:

ppt13 Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + NO GUIDE)

Participant 13 is not reflective of the group average. There’s no P3 effect, and the late positivity effect is actually reversed between conditions. There might even be a P2 effect in the orange condition. Oh dear. I wonder if this individual variation will get lost in the averaging process?

Here’s participant 15:

ppt15 Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + NO GUIDE)

Participant 15 shows the P3 effect, albeit about 100ms later than participant 9 does, but there isn’t really a late positivity here. Swings and roundabouts, innit.

However, despite this variation, if I average the three of them together, I get a waveform that is relatively close to the group average:

ppt9-13-15 Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + NO GUIDE)

The P3 effect is fairly clear, although the late positivity isn’t… but then again, it’s only from three participants, and EEG studies should generally use at least 20-25 participants. It would also be ideal if participants could do hundreds or thousands of trials so that the ERPs for each participant are much more reliable, but this experiment took an hour and a half as it is; nobody wants to sit in a chair strapped into a swimming cap full of electrodes for a whole day.

So, on the one hand, this shows that ERPs from a tenth of the sample size can actually be quite reflective of the group average ERPs… but on the other hand, this shows that even ERPs averaged over only three participants can still obscure the highly divergent readings of one of them.

Now, if only there were a way of calculating an average, knowing how accurate that average is, and also knowing what the variation in the sample size is like…

…which, finally, brings me onto the main point of this blog:

Why do we only plot the mean across all participants when we could also include measures of confidence and variance?

In behavioural data, it’s relatively common to plot line graphs where the line is the mean across participants, while there’s also a shaded area around the line which typically shows 95% confidence intervals. Graphs with confidence intervals look a bit like this (although normally a bit less like an earthworm with a go-faster stripe on it):

graph with CIs

This is pretty useful in visualising data. It’s taking a statistical measure of how reliable the measurement is, and plotting it in a way that’s easy to see.

So. Why aren’t ERPs plotted with confidence intervals? The obvious stumbling point is the ridiculous requirements of journals (see above), which would make the shading quite hard to do. But, if we all realised that everything happens on the internet now, where colour printing isn’t a thing, then we could plot and publish ERPs that look like this:

Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + 95pc CIs + NO GUIDE)

It’s nice, isn’t it? It also makes it fairly clear where the main effects are; not only do the lines diverge, the shaded areas do too. This might even go some way towards addressing Steve Levinson’s valid concerns about cognitive science data ignoring individual data… although only within one population. My data was acquired from 18-30 year old Dutch university students, and cannot be generalised to, say, 75 year old illiterate Hindi speakers with any degree of certainty, let alone 95%.

This isn’t really measuring the variance within a sample, though. How can we plot an ERP graph which gives some indication of how participant 13 had a completely different response from participants 9 and 15? Well, we could try plotting it with the shaded areas showing one standard deviation either side of the mean instead. It looks like this:

Cz electrode (RG onset timelock + SDs + NO GUIDE)

…which, let’s face it, is pretty gross. The colours overlap a lot, and it’s just kind of messy. But, it’s still informative; it indicates a fair chunk of the variation within my 29 participants, and it’s still fairly clear where the main effects are.

Is this a valid way of showing ERP data? I quite like it, but I’m not sure if other ERP researchers would find this useful (or indeed sensible). I’m also not sure if I’ve missed something obvious about this which makes it impractical or incorrect. It could well be that the amplitudes at each time point aren’t normally distributed, which would require some more advanced approaches to showing confidence intervals, but it’s something to go on at least.

I’d love to hear people’s opinions in the comments below.

To summarise, then:

– ERP graphs aren’t all that great

– but they could be if we plotted them logically

– and they could be really great if we plotted more than just the sample mean

Standard